Discussing Translanguaging: A Conversation Starter

Discussing Translanguaging: A Conversation Starter

Michael Rabbidge

To start the new year we’ve decided, as a team of bloggers, to take a slightly more integrated approach to our postings. We’ll start with a brief discussion on a topic of interest, and then take turns responding to each other to create a more shared learning approach with these blogs. We welcome you, as the reader, to join the discussion by posting questions in the comments section, and hopefully we’ll be able to answer these or provide more discussion in subsequent blogs.

That being said, I’ve decided to kick things off by discussing an area of research that I have had a real interest in since my days as teacher trainer in South Korea; that of language choice and use when teaching a foreign language.

I became interested in translanguaging as a concept as a result of my PhD thesis work, which looked at why and how South Korea elementary school teachers used English and Korean languages when teaching English. What really got me interested in this was the sense of guilt South Korean teachers tended to have when discussing the use of their mother tongue, Korean, to teach English. At that time, the Korean government had implemented a policy that prohibited the use of Korean while teaching English in the public school system, which led to a lot of confusion and conflict for these teachers.  

As this area of research continued, I came across the term translanguaging, which initially read like the more commonly known concept ‘code-switching’. Code switching is “understood by most informed scholars in a dynamic and creative fashion as the expressive transgression by bilingual speakers of their two separate languages, endows these speakers with agency and often finds in the very act of switching elements of linguistic mastery and virtuosity” (Otheguy, García, & Reid, 2015).

A definition of translanguaging that I like is “the deployment of a speaker’s full linguistic repertoire without regard for watchful adherence to the socially and politically defined boundaries of named (and usually national and state) languages” (Otheguy, García, & Reid, 2015).

When seen side by side, code-switching as a concept sees bi or multilingual speakers as switching between languages that exist separately in the mind of the speaker. Translanguaging on the other hand sees language existing as a single repertoire in the user’s mind, so that there is no switching between named languages, rather, the speaker reacts to changing contexts by employing contextually relevant elements of their single semiotic repertoire.

Furthermore, there is a greater socio-political focus within translanguaging research that extends it as a concept beyond that of code-switching.  Unpacking the quotes further reveals a shift in focus from linguistic competencies to that of linguistic repertoires which creates an opportunity to free speakers from monolingual ideologies and concerns of native-speakerism influences that have tended to negatively position learners of a new language or those born in certain geographical locations not traditionally associated with the target language. It also highlights socially constructed realties that have privileged the ‘one nation-one language’ ideology. In doing this, it is asking for an acknowledgement that multilingualism is the true linguistic norm of our world.

This interest has led me to employ a translanguaging perspective while conducting research on language use, and like any new concept, translanguaging continues to grow and inform understandings about effective teaching and learning practices in different contexts. There are a lot of directions that translanguaging can head in, and the better this concept is understood the more likely it can benefit a more socially inclusive language education environment. I look forward to further discussions on this topic, and if you have any questions feel free to reply to this blog.

The Science of Reading

The Science of Reading

The Science of Reading is a movement within educational circles to apply a more rigorous scientific method to understanding how people learn to read, so that teachers can be better informed about how to teach learners how to read. One definition explains the Science of Reading movement as “a corpus of objective investigation and accumulation of reliable evidence about how humans learn to read and how reading should be taught” (The International Literacy Association). This involves studying how reading operates, develops, is taught, shapes academic and cognitive growth, affects motivation and emotion, interacts with context, and impacts context in turn. It also includes genetic, biological, environmental, contextual, social, political, historical, and cultural factors that influence the acquisition and use of reading.

Within this movement there is debate regarding best practices and new areas of research inquiry, with one current debate centering on what constitutes scientific evidence, how much value should be placed on scientific evidence as opposed to other forms of knowledge, and how preservice teachers should be instructed to teach reading. This involves discussion around conflicting views in epistemology between constructivists and positivists on the basic mechanisms associated with reading development.

Constructivists insist that learners construct knowledge rather than just passively take in information, and a multitude of realties are constructed through lived experiences and interactions with others. This view sees reading as a natural act akin to learning a language, and emphasizes giving students enough opportunity to discover meaning through experiences in literacy-rich environment. It also encourages students to engage in a psycholinguistic guessing game in which readers use graphic, semantic, and syntactic knowledge to guess the meanings of printed words.

Contrasting this view is the post positivist view, a form of scientific study of the social world where the goal is to formulate abstract and universal laws of social universe and test these laws against collected data systematically to form an approximated understanding of reality. This view makes strong distinctions between innate language learning and effortful learning required to acquire reading skills, and argues for explicit instruction to help foster an understanding of how the written code is mapped onto language.

The Science of Reading provides compelling evidence for the teaching of reading that can be used to inform teachers of best practices, including but not limited to the following facts:

·    Orthographic depth impacts speed at which a language is learned to be read

·    Decoding skills and linguistic comprehension make independent contributions to the prediction of reading comprehension across diverse populations of readers

·    Decoding and linguistic comprehension account for almost all variance in reading comprehension

·    Importance of decoding skill in explaining variance in reading comprehension decreases across grades, importance of linguistic comprehension increases 

·    By the time students are in high school, linguistic comprehension and reading comprehension essentially form single dimension

·    Knowledge of alphabetic principle (i.e., how letters and sounds connect) and knowledge of morphophonemic nature of English necessary to create high-quality lexical representations essential to accurate and efficient decoding

·    Prior to formal reading instruction, young students develop phonological awareness and alphabet knowledge, other early literacy skills related to later decoding skills following formal reading instruction

·    Longitudinal studies indicate that linguistic comprehension skills from early childhood predict reading comprehension at end of elementary school

·    Prior to formal reading instruction, young students develop phonological awareness and alphabet knowledge, other early literacy skills related to later decoding skills following formal reading instruction

·    Longitudinal studies indicate that linguistic comprehension skills from early childhood predict reading comprehension at end of elementary school

Such a body of compelling evidence allows teachers to make informed decisions regarding effective practices. To discover more on the Science of Reading find the following reference. There is a wide fo variety of issues that the science of reading explores that will prove useful for both researchers and teachers alike.

Petscher, Y., Cabell, S.Q., Catts, H.W., Compton, D.L., Foorman, B.R., Hart, S.A., Lonigan, C.J., Phillips, B.M., Schatschneider, C., Steacy, L.M., Terry, N.P., & Wagner, R.K. (2020). How the Science of Reading Informs 21st-Century Education. Reading Research Quarterly, 55(S1), S267– S282. https://doi.org/10.1002/rrq.352